• Rezultati Niso Bili Najdeni

Od številčnosti do števil in aritmetike

6 Literatura

Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 278-291.

Ascher, M. & Ascher, R. (1997). Mathematics of the Incas: Code of the Quipu, 29-30.

Mineola, New York: Dover publications.

Bender, A. and Beller, S. (2011). Numerical Cognition and Ethnomathematics. V: D. B.

Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V. C. de Munck & M. D. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (270-289). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Bloch, M. (2012). Anthropology and the Cognitive Challenge. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Boesch, C. (2007). What Makes Us Human (Homo sapiens)? The Challenge of Cognitive Cross-Species Comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(3), 227-240.

Boesch, C. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzees and Human Cultures. Current Anthropology, 39(5), 591-614.

Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1533), 3281-3288.

Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. PNAS, 108, 10918-10925.

Butterworth, B., Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Numerical thought with and without words: Evidence from indigenous Australian children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(35), 13179-13184.

Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social Learning Mechanisms and Cumulative Cultural Evolution Is Imitation Necessary?. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1478-1483.

Campos, P. R., de Oliveira, V. M., Giro, R., & Galvão, D. S. (2004). Emergence of prime numbers as the result of evolutionary strategy. Physical review letters, 93(9), 098107.

Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge of number: Its evolution and ontogeny. Science, 282(5389), 641-642.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (2006). Geni, ljudstva in jeziki. Ljubljana: Krtina

Clements, D. H. (1999). Subitizing: What is it? Why teach it? Teaching children mathematics, 5, 400-405.

Coch, D., Dawson, G. & Fischer, K. eds. Human behavior, learning and the developing brain:

Atypical development. New York: Guilford Press.

Cohen, L. B., & Marks, K. S. (2002). How infants process addition and subtraction events.

Developmental Science, 5, 186–212.

Coolidge, F. L., & Overmann, K. A. (2012). Numerosity, abstraction, and the emergence of symbolic thinking. Current Anthropology, 53(2), 204-225.

d’Ambrosio, U. (1985). Ethnomathematics and its Place in the History and Pedagogy of Mathematics. For the learning of mathematics, 5(1), 44-48.

Darwin, C. (1859). The Origin of Species. London: Penguin Classics.

De Cruz, H. (2004). Why Humans Can Count Large Quantities Accurately. Philosophica Gent, 74, 63.

De Cruz, H. (2006). Towards a Darwinian approach to Mathematics. Foundations of science, 11(1-2), 157-196.

De Cruz, H. (2006). Why are some numerical concepts more successful than others? An evolutionary perspective on the history of number concepts. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 306-323.

De Cruz, H. (2008). An extended mind perspective on natural number representation.

Philosophical Psychology, 21(4), 475-490.

De Cruz, H. & De Smedt, J. (2013). Mathematical symbols as epistemic actions. Synthese, 190(1), 3-19.

Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B. & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the Social and Cognitive Processes Underlying Human Cumulative Culture. Science, 335, 1114-1118.

Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1), 1-42.

Dehaene, S. (2011). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, Revised and Updated Edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dehaene, S. (2003). Acalculia and number processing disorders. V: Feinberg, T. E. & Farah, M. J., eds., Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. McGraw-Hill, 2nd ed.

Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1991). Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1045-1074.

doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90076-K

Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic. Cortex. 33, 219-250.

Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L. (1998). Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends in neurosciences, 21(8), 355-361.

Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E., & Pica, P. (2008). Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science, 320(5880), 1217-1220.

Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 14, 218-224. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.008

Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive neuropsychology, 20(3-6), 487-506.

Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,122(3), 371.

Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of

mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284(5416), 970-974.

DeVore, I., & Tooby, J. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavoral evolution through strategic modelling. The evolution of human behavior: Primate models, 183-237.

Dictionary of Biology, Fifth Edition (2004). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Downey, G. & Lende, D. H. (2012). Neuroanthropology and the Enculturated Brain. V:

Downey, G. & Lende, D. H. (Eds.), The Enculturated Brain: an Introduction to Neuroanthropology (23-66). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Erdal, D., & Whiten, A. (1996). Egalitarianism and Machiavellian intelligence in human evolution. V: Modelling the early human mind, eds. P. Mellars & K. Gibson, 139-150.

Cambridge, MA: McDonald Institute Monographs.

Erdal, D., Whiten, A., Boehm, C., & Knauft, B. (1994). On human egalitarianism: an evolutionary product of Machiavellian status escalation? Current Anthropology. 35(2), 175-183.

Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Hauser, M. (2002). The representations underlying infants' choice of more: Object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychological Science, 13(2), 150-156.

Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(7), 307-314.

Fitch, W. T. (2010). The Evolution of Language. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.

Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2008). Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition, 108(3), 819-824.

Frank, M. C., Fedorenko, E., Lai, P., Saxe, R., & Gibson, E. (2012). Verbal interference suppresses exact numerical representation. Cognitive psychology, 64(1), 74-92.

Fry, D. P., & Söderberg, P. (2013). Lethal aggression in mobile forager bands and implications for the origins of war. Science, 341(6143), 270-273.

Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44(1), 43-74.

Geary, D. C. (2006). Development of mathematical understanding. V: Damon, W. (Gen. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th Ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science, 306(5695), 496-499.

Gould, S. J. (1992). Of Bamboos, Cicadas, and the Economy of Adam Smith. V: Ever since Darwin: Reflections in natural history. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company.

Hanakawa, T., Immisch, I., Toma, K., Dimyan, M. A., Van Gelderen, P., & Hallett, M.

(2003). Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89(2), 989-1002.

Hauser, M. D., Carey, S. & Hauser, L. B. (2000). Spontaneous Number Representation in Semi-Free-Ranging Rhesus Monkeys. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 267(1445), 829-833.

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569-1579.

Hauser, M. D., MacNeilage, P., & Ware, M. (1996). Numerical representations in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(4), 1514-1517.

Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 197-214.

Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreds, M. V., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis.

Science, 317, 1360-1366.

Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. Growing points in ethology, 303-317.

Ingold, T. (2007). The Trouble with ‘Evolutionary Biology’. Anthropology Today, 23(2), 13-17.

Jablonka, N, & Lamb, M. I. (2009). Štiri razsežnosti evolucije. Ljubljana: Zavod RS za šolstvo.

Kendal, J., Tehrani, J. J. & Odling-Smee, J. (2011). Human niche construction in interdisciplinary focus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 785-792.

Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. Cambridge, MA: The Museum.

Lakoff, G. & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics come from. How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Laland, K. N., & Sterelny, K. (2006). Perspective: seven reasons (not) to neglect niche construction. Evolution, 60(9), 1751-1762.

Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome:

bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11(2), 137-148.

Laland, K. N., Odling‐Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2001). Cultural niche construction and human evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 14(1), 22-33.

Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science, 14, 396–401.

Livingstone, M. S., Pettine, W. W., Srihasam, K., Moore, B., Morocz, I. A., & Lee, D.

(2014). Symbol addition by monkeys provides evidence for normalized quantity coding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(18), 6822-6827.

Malafouris, L. (2010). Grasping the concept of number: how did the sapient mind move beyond approximation?. V: Morley, I., & Renfrew, C. (2010). The archaeology of measurement: comprehending heaven, earth and time in ancient societies, 35-42.

Malafouris, L. (2013). How Things Shape the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Marks, J. (2011). The Alternative Introduction to Biological Anthropology. New York, NY:

Oxford University Press.

Marks, J. (2012). The Biological Myth of Human Evolution. Contemporary Social Sciences:

Journal of the Academy of Social Sciences, 7(3), 139-157.

Mazur, J. (2014). Notation, notation, notation: a brief history of mathematical symbols.

Pridobljeno s

http://www.theguardian.com/science/alexs-adventures-in-numberland/2014/may/21/notation-history-mathematical-symbols-joseph-mazur

McGrew, W. C. (2011). Kulturni šimpanz. Razmišljanja o kulturni primatologiji. Ljubljana:

Studia Humanitatis.

Mehler, J., & Bever, T. G. (1967). Cognitive capacity of very young children. Science, 158, 141–142.

Miura, I. T., Kim, C. C., Chang, C. M., & Okamoto, Y. (1988). Effects of language

characteristics on children’s cognitive representation of number: Cross-national comparisons.

Child Development, 59, 1445–1450.

Miura, I. T., Okamoto, Y., Kim, C. C., Steere, M., & Fayol, M. (1993). First graders’

cognitive representation of number and understanding of place value: Cross-national

comparisons—France, Japan, Korea, Sweden, and the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 24–30.

Molko, N., Cachia, A., Rivière, D., Mangin, J., Bruandet, M., LeBihan, D. L., & Dehaene, S.

(2004). Brain Anatomy in Turner Syndrome: Evidence for Impaired Social and Spatial-Numerical Networks. Cerebral Cortex. 14(8), 840-50.

Molko, N., Cachia, A., Rivière, D., Mangin, J., Bruandet, M., LeBihan, D. L., & Dehaene, S.

(2003). Functional and Structural Alterations of the Intraparietal Sulcus in a Developmental Dyscalculia of Genetic Origin. Neuron. 40(4), 847-858.

Moll, H. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Phil Trans R Soc B, 362, 639-648.

Monaghan, J. & Just, P. (2008). Zelo kratek uvod: Socialna in kulturna antropologija.

Ljubljana: Založba Krtina.

Odling-Smee, F.J. (2007). Niche inheritance: a possible basis for classifying multiple inheritance systems in evolution. Biol. Theor. 2, 276-289.

Piaget, J. (1965). The child’s conception of number. New York: Norton.

Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Bihan, D. L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus. Neuron. 44(3), 547-55.

doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014

Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306(5695), 499-503.

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and

language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107 (Supplement 2), 8993-8999.

Premack, D., & Premack, A. (2005). Evolution versus invention. Science, 307(5710), 673-673.

Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Richerson, P., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2003). Human Cooperation. Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation, 357-88. Cultural Evolution of Human Cooperation. V:

Hammerstein, P. (2003). Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rumbaugh, D. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and Hegel, M. T. (1987). Summation in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 13, 107-115.

Saxe, G. B. (1981). Body parts as numerals: A developmental analysis of numeration among the Oksapmin in Papua New Guinea. Child development, 306-316.

Saxe, G. B. (1982). The development of measurement operations among the Oksapmin of Papua New Guinea. Child Development, 53, 1242-1248.

Saxe, G. B., & Esmonde, I. (2004). Making change in Oksapmin tradestores: A study of shifting practices of quantification under conditions of rapid shift towards a cash economy. South Pacific Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 11-28.

Shettleworth, S. J. (2009). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(11), 477-481.

Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children.

Child Development, 75, 428–444.

Simon, H. (1996). The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sperber, D, & Hirschfeld, L. (1999). Culture, Cognition, and Evolution. In Robert Wilson &

Frank Keil (eds) The MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, str. cxi-cxxxii

Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., Moortele, P. V., Bihan, D. L., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S.

(2000). Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: A brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation. Brain. 123, 2240-55.

Štrus, J., Zrimec, Z. & Polak, (2001). Mali leksikon zoologije. Ljubljana, Tehniška založba Slovenije.

Tang, Y., Zhang, W., Chen, K., Feng, S., Ji, Y., Shen, J., Reiman, E. M. & Liu, Y. (2006).

Arithmetic Processing in the Brain Shaped by Culture. PNAS, 103(28), 10775-780.

Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 364(1528), 2405-2415.

Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and brain sciences, 16(03), 495-511.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and brain sciences, 28(05), 675-691.

Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation: The Interdependence Hypothesis. Current

Anthropology, 53(6), 673-692.

Varley, R. A., Klessinger, N. J. C., Romanowski, C. A. J. & Siegal, M. (2005). Agrammatic but Numerate. PNAS, 102(9), 3519-3524.

Vygotsky, L. (1991). S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ward, C., & Smuts, B. B. (2007). Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Animal Cognition, 10(1), 71-80.

Whiten, A. & Erdal, D. E. (2012). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2119-2129.

Wilson, R. A. & Kiel, C. K. (1999). The MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155-193.

Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358(6389), 749-750.